Studies on the use of irony: an affinity selector mechanism
Abstract
Even if pragmatics has studied irony in depth, there is surprisingly little agreement not only to
its functions but also to its basic definition. Irony appears to be the result of different
linguistics acts (refusing, echoing, pretending, alluding), that in any case serve the speaker’s
goal to highlight an incongruity between some previous expectations and the current
situation. However, there seems to be something else that characterizes irony so as to render
it unique, something that makes it even more distinctively different from other figures of
speech, from other rhetorical strategies or structural devices.
The paper proposes a wider vision of investigation that takes into account the fact that irony
is a relational strategy in the sense that it operates not only between meanings (said and
unsaid) but also between people. Irony, far from being only a way of communicating a
proposition, is a sophisticated interaction that takes place between highly social beings. To be
understood, it requires contextual and metacommunicative elements, and also mutual
attribution of knowledge, intentions and affective states between speaker and listener.
Defining what the ironic linguistic meaning is, allows us to understand just only one aspect of
this complex phenomenon, but it doesn’t allow us to understand that irony is an attitude, a
way of doing things in order to create a relationship, of inclusion as much as exclusion, with
others. Understanding the social aspects of irony is a challenge for pragmatics, that allows us
to have an approach not only limited to understanding disparity between literal and figurative
meaning but which investigates irony as a social phenomenon that brings together several
different cognitive social abilities.
References
Anolli, Luigi, Ciceri, Rita, Infantino, Maria G. (2000), «Irony as a game of Implicitness: Acoustic profiles of ironic communication», in Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, n. 29(3), pp. 275-311.
Anolli, Luigi, Ciceri, Rita, Infantino, Maria G. (2002), «From “blame by praise” to “praise by blame”: Analysis of vocal pattern in ironic communication», in International Journal of Psychology, n. 37, pp. 266-276.
Attardo, Salvatore, Eisterhod, Jodi, Hay, Jennifer, Poggi, Isabella (2003), «Multimodal markers of irony and sarcasm», in Humor. International Journal of humor research, n. 16(2), pp. 243-60.
Bryant, Gregory A., Fox Tree, Jean E. (2005), «Is there an ironic tone of voice?», in Language and Speech, n. 48, pp. 257-277.
Colston, Herbert L. (1997), «Salting a wound or sugaring a pill: The pragmatic functions of ironic criticism», in Discourse processes, n. 23, pp. 24-53.
Coulmas, Florian (1981), edited by, Conversational Routine: Explorations in Standardized Communicative situations and prepatterned speech, The Hague, Mouton.
Coupland, Justine (2000), Small Talk. Language in social life, Routledge, New York.
Dews, Shelley, Kaplan, Joan, Winner, Ellen (1995), «Why not to say directly? The social functions of irony», in Discourse Processes, n. 19, pp. 347-367.
Dynel, Marta (2013), «Irony from a neo-Gricean perspective: On untruthfulness and evalutative implicature», in Intercultural Pragmatics, n. 10(3), pp. 403-431.
Filippova, Eva, Astington, Janet Wilde (2008), «Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding», in Child Development, n. 79(1), pp. 126-138.
Giora, Rachel (2003), On our mind: Salience, context and figurative language, Oxford University Press, New York.
Grice, Paul H. (1957), «Meaning», in The Philosophical review, n. 64, pp. 377-388.
Grice, Paul H. (1975), Logic and conversation: The William James Lectures, II, in Cole P., Morgan J. L., eds, Syntax and Semantics 3: Speech acts, Academic Press, New York, pp. 41-58 (trad. it. Logica e Conversazione, in Iacona A., Paganini E., a cura di, Filosofia del linguaggio, Raffaello Cortina, Milano, 2003, pp. 221-44).
Grice, Paul H. (1989) [1978], Further notes on logic and conversation, in Grice H. P., edited by, Studies on the way of words, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp. 41-57.
Happé, Francesca (1993), «Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: a test of relevance theory», in Cognition, n. 48(2), pp. 101-119.
Jared, Debra, Pandolfo, Alyssa (2021), «The effect of speaker age on the perception of ironic insults», in Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, n. 75(2), pp. 146-154.
Katz, Albert N., Blasko, Dawn, G. Kazmerski, Victoria A. (2004), «Saying What you don’t mean. Social influences on sarcastic language processing», in Current directions in Phychological Science, n. 13(5), pp. 186-189.
Katz, Albert N., Pexman, Penny (1997), «Interpreting figurative statements: Speaker occupation can change metaphor to irony», in Metaphor and Symbol, n. 12(1), pp. 19-41.
Langdon, Robyn, Davies, Martin, Coltheart, Max (2002), «Understanding minds and understanding communicated meanings in schizophrenia», in Mind & Language, n. 17(1‐2), pp. 68-104.
Li, Xiaoming, Die Hu, Wenrui Deng, Qian Tao, Ying Hu, Xiaoxue Yang, Zheng Wang, Rui Tao, Lizhuang Yang & Xiaochu Zhang (2017), «Pragmatic ability deficit in schizophrenia and associated theory of mind and executive function», in Frontiers in psychology, n. 8, 2164.
Lucariello, Joan (1994), «Situational Irony: A concept of Events gone away», in Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, n.123, pp. 129-145.
Martin, Ingerigh, McDonald, Skye (2004), «An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome», in Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, n. 34(3), pp. 311-328.
Mascaro, Olivier, Sperber, Dan (2009), «The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children’s vigilance towards deception», in Cognition, n.112(3), pp. 367-380.
Mazzarella, Diana, Pouscoulous N. (2021), «Pragmatics and epistemic vigilance: A developmental perspective», in Mind and Language, n. 36, pp. 355-376.
Mo, Shuliang, Su, Yanjie, Chan, Raymond C., Liu, Jianxin (2008), «Comprehension of metaphor and irony in schizophrenia during remission: the role of theory of mind and IQ», in Psychiatry Research, n. 157, pp. 21-29.
Origgi, Gloria (2013), «Epistemic injustice and epistemic trust», in Social Epistemology: A journal of Knowledge, Culture and Policy, n. 26(2), pp. 221-235.
Padilla Cruz, Manuel (2004), On the social importance of phatic utterances: Some considerations for a relevance-theoretic approach, in Garcés Conejos P., Gomez Moron R., Fernandez Amaya L., Padilla Cruz M., eds, Current Trends in Intercultural, Cognitive and Social Pragmatics, Research Group Intercultural Pragmatic Studies, Sevilla, pp. 199-216.
Padilla Cruz, Manuel (2005), «On the phatic interpretation of utterances: a complementary relevance-theoretic approach», in Revista Alicantina de Estudios Ingleses, n. 18, pp. 227-246.
Padilla Cruz, Manuel (2007), Metarepresentations and Phatic Utterances: A pragmatic proposal about the generation of solidarity between interlocutors, in Cap P., Nijakowska J., eds., Current Trends in Pragmatics, Cambridge Scholars Publishing, New Castle, pp. 110-28.
Padilla Cruz, Manuel (2008), Can irony be phatic? A relevance-theoretic proposal, in Friend Losada M. et al., eds, Proceedings of the 30th International AEDEAN Conference, CD rom edition, Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Huelva, Huelva.
Pexman, Penny M. (2005), Social factors in the interpretation of verbal irony: The roles of speaker and listener characteristics, in Herbert L., Colston, Katz Albert N., eds, Figurative language comprehension: Social and cultural influences, Psychology Press, New York and Hove, pp. 209-232.
Pexman, Penny M., Glenwright, Melanie, Hala, Suzanne, Kowbel, Stacey L., Jungen, Sara (2006), «Children's use of trait information in understanding verbal irony», in Metaphor and Symbol, n. 21(1). pp. 39-60.
Pexman Penny M., Olineck, Kara M. (2002), «Understanding Irony: How Do Stereotypes cue speaker intent?», in Journal of Language and Social Psychology, n. 21(3), pp. 245-274.
Regel, Stefanie, Coulson, Seana, Gunter, Thomas C. (2010), «The communicative style of a speaker can affect language comprehension? ERP evidence from the comprehension of irony», in Brain research, n. 1311, pp. 121-35.
Schneider, Klaus P. (1988), Small Talk. Analysing phatic Discourse, Hitzeroth, Marburg.
Scollon, Ron, Scollon, Suzanne W. (1995), Intercultural Communication. A discourse Approach, Blackwell, Cambridge
Searle, John R. (1976), «A classification of Illocutionary Acts Language», in Society, vol. 5 n.1, pp.1-23.
Senft, Gunter (2009), «Phatic communion», in Culture and language use, n. 2, pp. 226-233.
Sperber, Dan (1984), «Verbal irony: Pretense or Echoic Mention?», in Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, n. 113, pp. 130-136.
Sperber, Dan, Clément, Fabrice, Heintz, Christophe, Mascaro, Olivier, Mercier, Hugo, Origgi, Gloria, Wilson, Deirdre (2010), «Epistemic vigilance», in Mind & Language, n. 25(4). 359-393.
Sperber, Dan, Wilson, Deirdre (1981), «Irony and the use-mention distinction», in Philosophy, n. 3, pp. 143-184.
Spotorno, Nicola, Koun, Eric, Prado,Jérôme, Van Der Henst, Jean-Baptiste, Noveck Ira A. (2012), «Neural evidence that utterance-processing entails mentalizing: The case of irony», in NeuroImage, n. 63(1), pp. 25-39.
Spotorno, Nicola, Cheylus, Anne, Van Der Henst, Jean-Baptiste, Noveck, Ira A. (2013), «What’s behind a P600? Integration Operations during Irony Processing», in PlosOne, n. 8(6), e66839.
Sperber, Dan, Wilson, Deirdre (1986/1995), Relevance. Cognition and communication, Blackwell Publishers Ltd, Oxford.
Sullivan, Kate, Winner, Ellen, Hopfield, Natalie (1995), «How children tell a lie from a joke: The role of second‐order mental state attributions», in British Journal of Developmental Psychology, n. 13(2), pp. 191–204.
Sullivan, Kate, Winner, Ellen, Tager-Flusberg, Helen (2003), «Can adolescents with Williams Syndrome tell the difference between lies and jokes?», in Developmental Neurppsychology, n. 23(1-2), pp. 85-103.
Utsumi, Akira (2000), «Verbal irony as implicit display of ironic enviroment: Distinguishing ironic utterances from nonirony», in Journal of Pragmatics, n. 32, pp. 1777-1806.
Wilson, Deirdre (2006), «The pragmatics of verbal irony: Echo or pretense?», in Lingua, n. 116, pp. 1722-1743.
Wilson, Deirdre (2009), «Irony and metarepresentation», in UCL Working papers in Linguistics, n. 21, pp. 183-226.
Wilson, Deirdre (2013), «Irony comprehension: A developmental perspective», in Journal of Pragmatic, n. 59, pp. 40-56.
Wilson, Deirdre, Sperber, Dan (2012), Meaning and relevance, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Winner, Ellen, Leekam, Sue (1991), «Distinguishing irony from deception: Understanding the speaker's second‐order intention», in British Journal of Developmental Psychology, n. 9(2), pp. 257-270.
Yus, Francisco (2016), «Propositional attitude, affective attitude and irony comprehension», in Pragmatics & Cognition, n. 23(1), pp. 92-116.
Zegarak Vlad, Clark, Billy (1999), «Phatic Interpretations and phatic communication», in Journal of Linguistics, n. 35(2), pp. 321-346.
Works published in RIFL are released under Creative Commons Licence:Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.